After you review a flashcard, your ability to recall that information declines gradually unless you see it again. Psychologists often model this decline as an exponential decay. The forgetting rate can vary by subject and by individual, but the pattern is remarkably consistent. Spaced repetition systems take advantage of this by scheduling reviews just before you’re likely to forget, strengthening your memory each time.
The study of memory decay goes back to Hermann Ebbinghaus in the late nineteenth century. By testing his own ability to recall nonsense syllables, he plotted the famous forgetting curve: a rapid drop in recall shortly after learning followed by a slower decline over time. Modern research has refined his model but the core idea remains: without reinforcement, memories fade. Understanding this natural trajectory is the first step toward building an effective study plan.
If your retention after the first study session is (expressed as a decimal), and the daily forgetting rate is , then after days your recall is . This calculator simplifies things by using percentages. You input your expected forgetting rate each day and the time until your next review. The result shows how much of the material you’ll likely remember when you sit down to study again.
The exponential model captures two important features of memory. First, the rate of loss is proportional to what you currently remember, so the first day after learning sees the steepest drop. Second, the curve never quite reaches zero, reflecting the fact that traces of the memory may linger for years. Repeated reviews push the curve upward and flatten it, meaning each subsequent forgetting episode is slower than the last.
Spaced repetition is a learning technique that schedules reviews at increasing intervals. Each time you successfully recall a card, the system waits a little longer before showing it again. This approach mirrors how memory strengthens with use. By revisiting information just before it slips away, you reinforce the neural connections and require less effort for future retrieval.
Many popular study apps implement spaced repetition algorithms. Some use fixed schedules, while others adapt dynamically based on your performance. Regardless of the platform, the underlying principle is the same: spacing reviews beats cramming. Understanding the decay equation helps you fine-tune these schedules so they align with your personal forgetting rate and the goals of your study session.
This tool translates the mathematical model into actionable numbers. Start by entering your initial retention—the percentage you remember immediately after studying. Next, estimate the daily forgetting rate. A rate of 15% means you lose 15% of the remaining memory each day you do not review. Then specify how many days will pass before your next study session. The calculator outputs your expected recall at that time.
The optional field for desired retention adds another layer. Suppose you want to stay above 80% recall. The calculator computes how many days can pass before your memory dips below that threshold. This feature helps plan study intervals: review sooner for higher retention, or accept a lower percentage to free up time for new material.
All computations happen in your browser—no data is stored or transmitted. You can experiment freely without worrying about privacy.
How do you know your initial retention or forgetting rate? Start with rough estimates. If you tend to recall most of a lesson right away, 90% is reasonable. Forgetting rates vary widely: simple facts might decay at 5% per day while complex formulas vanish at 20% without review. Track your performance over several study sessions and adjust the numbers until the predictions match your experience.
Your desired retention level depends on your goals. Language learners often aim for 80–90% to keep vocabulary fresh. Medical students reviewing thousands of terms might settle for 70% on older cards to make room for new material. There is no universal “right” number; the best threshold balances mastery with available time.
Imagine you finish learning a batch of vocabulary cards with a 90% immediate recall rate, and your personal forgetting rate is about 15% per day. If you wait three days to review, your projected recall is , which equals roughly 65%. If your desired retention is 80%, the calculator will tell you to review within about 1.5 days instead. Adjusting the interval illustrates the trade‑off between study frequency and memory strength.
Try experimenting with different scenarios. Set the forgetting rate to 5% and the interval to seven days: the predicted recall jumps above 70%. On the other hand, increase the forgetting rate to 25% and the same interval yields less than 25% recall. These comparisons reveal why difficult subjects demand closer review while easier topics can be revisited less often.
Use the calculator to schedule sessions more strategically. If you know a busy week is coming, plug in your numbers to see how long you can safely wait before review. You can also prioritize cards with high forgetting rates, ensuring they appear more frequently. Over time, track the intervals that produce successful recall and adjust the forgetting rate parameter to reflect your personal curve.
Many learners pair this quantitative approach with qualitative notes. When a card feels shaky, lower the assumed retention or increase the forgetting rate. When a concept clicks instantly, do the opposite. The goal is not perfection but continuous calibration so the schedule fits your mind.
The model assumes a constant forgetting rate, yet real memory is influenced by sleep, stress, context, and the depth of your original learning. Review sessions themselves can vary in quality: a distracted glance at a flashcard reinforces less than an active recall attempt. Use the numbers as guideposts, not absolute truths. If results consistently differ from predictions, adjust the parameters or seek alternative study methods.
Do I need to know calculus to use this? No. The calculator handles the math. You only provide the percentages and days.
What forgetting rate should I start with? Begin with 10–15%. After a few rounds of study, compare the predicted recall with actual performance and tweak the rate accordingly.
Can I use this for subjects other than flashcards? Yes. Any discrete piece of information that benefits from periodic review—equations, historical dates, anatomical terms—can fit the model.
Does the calculator store my data? No. Everything runs locally in your browser. Refresh the page to clear your inputs.
Memory is dynamic, but with a bit of planning you can stay ahead of the forgetting curve. This calculator offers a simple way to visualize how quickly knowledge slips away and how small adjustments to your study schedule can preserve it. By experimenting with initial retention, forgetting rates, and desired thresholds, you build intuition about your own learning process. Use these insights to craft a review routine that keeps information fresh without overwhelming your calendar.
Create an efficient study plan using spaced repetition. Enter days until your exam and topics to get a suggested review schedule.
Estimate vocabulary retention based on time since study and daily retention rate.
Determine the retention factor of analytes in chromatography using retention and dead times.